To understand downranking, we must first understand Halliday's rank scale, as follows:
Clause Now, downranking occurs when something from a higher rank functions at a lower rank. For us, we will be mainly concerned with downranked clauses. So, if a clause functions as something lower than a clause (say, a participant or part of a participant), we consider it a downranked clause. Some examples are:
In (1) and (2), it is quite easy to see downranking at work. The underlined segments are technically clauses. But because they function as subjects, they are said to be nominalised and, therefore, downranked. Sometimes, we encounter stuff like (3). The underlined segment is traditionally labelled a restrictive (or defining) relative clause. A restrictive relative clause is downranked because it is part of the NG. As opposed to restrictive relative clauses, the following in (4) is a non-restrictive (or non-defining) relative clause:
Please note that non-restrictive relative clauses are NOT downranked. Instead, they serve as a kind of descriptive gloss to the independent clause and are regarded as a subordinate clause. So, to repeat: restrictive relative clauses are downranked clauses, but non-restrictive relative clauses are ranking clauses. But how can we tell a restrictive from a non-restrictive relative clause? Ah ... Halliday & Matthiessen say: "As far as their expression is concerned, non-defining relative clauses are clearly signalled both in speech and in writing. In written English, a non-defining relative clause is marked off by punctuation—usually commas, but sometimes by being introduced with a dash; whereas a defining relative clause is not separated by punctuation from its antecedent. This, in turn, reflects the fact that in spoken English, whereas a defining relative clause enters into a single tone group together with its antecedent, a non-defining relative [clause] forms a separate tone group." (Halliday & Matthiessen 2014: 467) If you ask me, this criterion is a little flimsy. But let's leave it at that—it is a simple test and it works well in most cases. To make things easy for you, a downranked clause can only occur as the following (which means that if you see something that doesn't fit the following list, you don't have a downranked clause):
Also, all the noun clauses you agonised over in your first-year modules are downranked clauses, except those that come after verbs of thinking and saying. Hence, in the following, we have a downranked clause: "Hearing Alvin's snores is quite an experience." But in the following, we have two separate clauses
Here are more examples of downranking, some involving restrictive relative clauses, and others, you wish you never knew. We will follow the convention on page 1 and mark downranked clauses as [[...]].
Admittedly, identifying a downranked clause isn't always easy. Most cases involve relative clauses, such as (3). But you already know a useful criterion you could use to tell apart a restrictive from a non-restrictive relative clause ... right? The grand-daddy of all headaches involves clauses coming after mental processes (oops ... if you have no idea what these are, please go to the Transitivity page ... sorry!). How do we analyse, for example, (13) and (14)?
To help you along, here's a brief list of probes you might want to use to see if there is a downranked clause lurking anywhere. Subject possible? Look at the underlined segments in (13) and (14). A ranking clause, precisely because it is a separate clause and not a participant, cannot serve as the subject. A downranked clause, on the other hand, can serve as the subject in the passive variant of the clause (some downranked clauses are part of a word group, but this isn't the case here). Here are the results of this probe:
This involves the use of cleft constructions, which take the following form: It + be + focus + non-restrictive relative clause Now, ranking clauses cannot be the focus in cleft constructions, simply because they are not participants. On the other hand, downranked clauses can, provided they are not part of a word group. So:
Substitution possible? (A word of caution—this test is the weakest among the five listed here. It doesn't always give good results.) A projected, ranking clause can be substituted by the word so. A downranked clause, on the other hand, can't; it can only be substituted by reference items such as that, it, etc. Hence:
Direct thought possible? If the underlined segment is a full ranking clause, we should be able to cast it in the form of direct thought, but not if otherwise:
"The fact" possible? (This is the most convenient test—it is often used for a quick result.) Try inserting "the fact" before the respective segments. Only downranked clauses can serve as a postmodifier after "the fact":
From the various probes above, there are two ranking clauses in (13), but only one in (14), which gives us:
|
Page-internal links
![]() Panadol Restrictive rel clause Comparison Result (Other) Enhancement Is there more? Sample analysis Online quiz Back to page 1 Halliday, M.A.K., & Matthiessen, C.M.I.M. (2014). Halliday's Introduction to Functional Grammar, 4th ed. London: Routledge. Martin, J.R., Matthiessen, C.M.I.M., & Painter, C. (1997). Working with Functional Grammar (pp. 179-183). London: Arnold. |